Why can’t we tickle ourselves ?

By Luke Toones

Have you ever wondered why tickling yourself doesn’t produce the same sensation as when someone else does it? This curious phenomenon reveals fascinating insights about how our brain processes sensory information and why self-tickling fails to elicit the giggles we experience when tickled by others.

The science behind failed self-tickling attempts

When you run your fingers along the sole of your foot or touch your own armpit, you likely feel little more than a simple touch. However, when someone else performs the exact same action, it can trigger an uncontrollable ticklish response. This difference isn’t just psychological—it’s deeply rooted in our brain’s predictive mechanisms.

Your brain constantly generates predictions about incoming sensory experiences. When you initiate a movement, your brain simultaneously sends two signals: one to your muscles to execute the action, and another—called an “efference copy”—to regions like the cerebellum that process sensory information. This efference copy allows your brain to anticipate and prepare for sensory feedback, creating an internal simulation of expected sensations.

Research confirms this theory through clever experimental designs. In one notable study, participants attempted to tickle themselves using a robotic arm that precisely replicated their movements. When the robot’s actions perfectly matched their own, participants reported minimal ticklish sensations. However, when researchers introduced slight delays or directional changes, the tickling sensation intensified significantly.

Tickling ScenarioPredictabilityTicklish Response
Self-ticklingHighly predictableMinimal
Robot-tickling (synchronized)PredictableSlight
Robot-tickling (delayed)Less predictableModerate
Other person ticklingUnpredictableStrong

Brain imaging studies further support these findings. When participants received identical tactile stimulation from themselves versus an external source, their somatosensory cortex—the brain region processing touch—showed significantly higher activity during external stimulation compared to self-generated touch.

How sensory attenuation shapes our perception

The phenomenon behind self-tickling failure—called sensory attenuation—plays a crucial role in how we navigate our environment. This filtering mechanism helps our limited attentional capacity focus on unexpected and potentially important stimuli while downplaying predictable sensations generated by our own actions.

This attenuation process affects multiple sensory systems beyond touch. Consider vision: despite constant eye movements, we perceive a stable world rather than a jumpy one. Our brain filters out self-generated visual changes by predicting and canceling the sensory consequences of our eye movements.

You can experience this yourself with a simple experiment: close one eye and gently press against your open eye through the eyelid. The world appears to shift, not because the environment moved, but because you created eye movement without the corresponding efference copy that normally suppresses such visual shifts.

Sensory attenuation also explains why we often underestimate the force we exert when touching objects or other people. This perceptual bias can lead to unintentional force escalation in playful interactions between children. Each child underestimates their own applied force while perceiving their playmate’s actions as stronger, causing both to gradually increase their force in a misguided attempt to match what they believe they’re receiving.

The brain’s predictive capabilities serve several important functions:

  • Filtering redundant sensory information
  • Distinguishing between self-generated and external stimuli
  • Allowing rapid error correction in movements
  • Maintaining perceptual stability despite constant sensory input changes
  • Conserving limited cognitive resources for novel or unexpected events

Beyond tickling: neural prediction and neurological disorders

The mechanisms that prevent self-tickling may provide valuable insights into certain neurological and psychiatric conditions. When the brain’s predictive systems malfunction, various perceptual abnormalities can emerge.

Interestingly, people with schizophrenia often demonstrate reduced sensory attenuation and can sometimes tickle themselves more effectively than neurotypical individuals. This finding aligns with the symptom of feeling disconnected from one’s own actions or experiencing them as externally controlled.

The same predictive models may help explain auditory hallucinations, where internal thought processes fail to be properly attenuated and are instead perceived as external voices. The brain’s ability to differentiate self-generated from external stimuli appears compromised in these conditions.

Our understanding of these mechanisms continues to evolve as neuroscience research advances. The humble question of why we can’t tickle ourselves has spawned research with far-reaching implications for understanding human perception and neurological functioning.

The evolutionary advantage of tickling immunity

From an evolutionary perspective, the inability to tickle oneself likely serves important adaptive functions. Ticklish areas typically correspond to vulnerable body parts that would benefit from heightened protection against external threats.

The tickle response encourages social bonding through shared laughter while simultaneously training defensive reactions to potential dangers. By filtering out self-generated tickling sensations, our nervous system maintains sensitivity to unexpected touch that might signal genuine threats.

This selective attention mechanism helps explain why sensory experiences often differ based on their source. Our brain’s remarkable predictive capabilities extend beyond tickling to influence countless aspects of perception, from how we taste food to how we interpret social interactions.

The next time someone manages to tickle you despite your best defenses, appreciate the sophisticated neural mechanisms at work. Your uncontrollable laughter reveals not just personal sensitivity, but a fundamental aspect of how your brain filters and processes sensory information—prioritizing the unexpected while dampening the predictable to create our seamless experience of the world.

About the author
Luke Toones
Published in